Group Grade Grubbing versus Cooperative LEARNING

image_print

 

EDUCATIONAL LEADERSHIP
March 1991

Group Grade Grubbing versus Cooperative Learning

By Alfie Kohn

Even before the recent surge of interest in cooperative learning (CL), researchers and practitioners were already staking out positions on precisely what the term denotes and how the idea should be implemented.  Constructive controversies (or, less charitably, factional disputes) have arisen with respect to almost every aspect of CL theory and practice.  Everyone in the field agrees that students benefit when they can help each other to learn instead of having to work against each other or apart from each other; beyond this, unanimity is in short supply.

What should be one of the central areas of discussion, however, has not yet received the attention it deserves.  I refer to the prominent role assigned to grades, awards, certificates, and other rewards in many of the CL models now being offered to teachers.  While some approaches incorporate these rewards without calling attention to that fact, others assert that rewards are the linchpin of cooperation.  Some writers even go so far as to use the phrases “cooperative goals” and “cooperate reward structures” interchangeably.

Most researchers would agree, I think, that effective CL depends on helping students to develop what the social psychologist Morton Deutsch (1949) called “promotive interdependence,” in which the goals of group members are positively linked and their interactions are characterized by mutual facilitation.  (Counterbalancing this in most versions of CL is some feature to assure individual accountability so that each student is held responsible to an external source for participating in the process and for learning.)  But the assumption that interdependence is best achieved — or even, as some would have it, that it can only be achieved — by the use of rewards is a claim that ought to have been critically examined long ago.

A significant body of research in social psychology has shown that rewards, often known as “extrinsic motivators” because the inducement is something other than the task itself, are not only surprisingly limited in their effectiveness but actually tend to undermine interest in the task and, over the long run, to reduce the quality of many kinds of performance.  It is possible that this literature is simply unfamiliar to most CL partisans and other educators — indeed, the key citations rarely appear in bibliographies — or it may be that their relevance to the classroom has not been fully appreciated.

Personally, I managed to erect an impermeable barrier between these two issues, a sort of cognitive Berlin Wall that remained in place for quite some time.  I lectured and wrote about CL, asserting that grades were key to creating student interdependence, paying no attention to the cautionary studies on extrinsic motivators that I had written about in other contexts.  My refusal to consider how the latter bears on the former may have reflected a dim understanding of how this recognition would demand a radical and troubling reconsideration of CL itself.

That no artificial inducement can offer the motivational power of intrinsic interest in a task seems undeniable.  Think of someone whom you regard as extraordinarily good at what he or she does for a living; then ask yourself whether this individual is concerned primarily to collect a paycheck.  Finding enjoyment in what one does is characteristic of most people who reach for excellence.  The same is true of students in the classroom.

But the “hidden costs of rewards” (Lepper and Greene 1978) have to do not only with their relative lack of efficacy but with their positively corrosive effect on both attitude and performance.  The psychologist Robert J. Sternberg (1990) recently summarized what virtually all motivation researchers now concede:  “Nothing tends to undermine creativity quite like extrinsic motivators do.  They also undermine intrinsic motivation: when you give extrinsic rewards for certain kinds of behavior, you tend to reduce children’s interest in performing those behaviors for their own sake” (p. 144).  More succinctly, rewards have been described as the “enemies of exploration” (Condry 1977).

Despite the continuing influence of Skinnerian psychology on education and on lay thinking, this phenomenon is not entirely counterintuitive.  The following three-step sequence of events will sound all too familiar to many of us:  (1) We engage in some activity simply because it is pleasurable, (2) we get paid for doing it, and (3) we suddenly find ourselves unwilling to do it unless we are paid.  We have come to see ourselves as working in order to receive the reward — in this case, money — with the result that our interest in the activity has mysteriously evaporated along the way.

This effect has been documented repeatedly, beginning in the early 1970s with the research of Mark Lepper at Stanford University (for an early summary, see Lepper and Greene 1975), Edward Deci at the University of Rochester (Deci and Ryan 1985), and their respective students.  Since then, other researchers replicating and clarifying the phenomenon include John Nicholls (1989), Judith M. Harackiewicz (e.g., Harackiewicz, Manderlink, and Sansone 1984), Mark Morgan in Ireland (1983, 1984), and Ruth Butler in Israel (Butler and Nisan 1986, Butler 1987, 1988, 1989).  Their experiments have shown, inter alia, that preschoolers who are told they will receive an award for drawing with felt-tip markers subsequently show less interest in using them (Greene and Lepper 1974); that college students competing to solve a puzzle are less likely to continue working on such puzzles than are those who had not competed (Deci et al. 1981); that merely watching someone else get rewarded for doing a task is enough to reduce one’s own motivation to do it (Morgan 1983); that the expectation of being evaluated distracts one from the task at hand and interferes with involvement and interest in it (Harackiewicz, Manderlink, and Sansone 1984); and that not only grades but even some kinds of praise (as opposed to purely informational feedback) can undermine interest in an activity (Ryan 1982, Butler 1987).

In addition to these studies, whose dependent variable is motivation, Teresa Amabile at Brandeis University and other researchers have shown that rewards often lead to lower performance, particularly at creative tasks.  For example,

* students promised a reward if they were effective at tutoring younger children took longer to communicate ideas, got frustrated more easily, and ended up with pupils who didn’t understand as well a group of children who learned from tutors promised no reward (Garbarino 1975),

* children and undergraduates who expected to receive a prize for making collages or telling stories proved to be less imaginative at both tasks than those who received nothing (Amabile, Hennessey, and Grossman 1986),

* when creative writers were asked to spend a few minutes reflecting on extrinsic reasons for writing — making money, impressing teachers, and so forth — their poetry dropped in quality and also was judged to be worse than the poems written by people who weren’t thinking about these things (Amabile 1985),

* teenagers offered a reward for remembering details about a newspaper story they had recently read had poorer recall than those who received nothing for their efforts; moreover, they also scored lower on two measures of creativity (Kruglanski, Friedman, and Zeevi 1971).

All of these studies have direct implications for classroom learning, but other research has shown that the destructive effects of rewards extend to other spheres:  They are counterproductive for promoting generosity and other prosocial behavior (see a review in Kohn 1990), for eliciting love toward one’s romantic partner (Seligman, Fazio, and Zanna 1980), and for motivating employees to use seat belts (Geller et al. 1987).  In short, the conclusion offered for one experiment seems an apt summary of an entire body of research:  “The more salient the reward the more undermining of performance [is] observed” (Condry 1977, p. 464).

Among the explanations proposed to account for these remarkably consistent findings are these:  First, people who think of themselves as working for a reward feel controlled by it, and this lack of self-determination interferes with creativity (Deci and Ryan 1985).  Second, rewards encourage “ego involvement” to the exclusion of “task involvement,” and the latter is more predictive of achievement (Nicholls 1989).  Third, the promise of a reward is “tantamount to declaring that the activity is not worth doing for its own sake” (A. S. Neill, quoted in Morgan 1984); indeed, anything construed as a prerequisite to some other goal will likely be devalued as a result (Lepper et al. 1982).

All of these explanations account for reduced performance on the basis of how rewards reduce interest in the given task.  But the decline of interest and the decline in performance are distinct phenomena, each significant in itself.  The reduction in motivation also has undesirable effects on “self-esteem, perceived cognitive competence, and sense of control” (Ryan, Connell, and Deci, 1985, p. 45); it is undesirable apart from its achievement effects.  Conversely, extrinsic inducements may also reduce creativity for a reason having nothing to do with intrinsic motivation:  they encourage students to work as quickly as possible, take few risks, and focus narrowly on a task.  A reward-driven child (or adult) is after the goodie, and this mental set is hardly conducive to the playful encounter with words or numbers or ideas that characterizes true creativity (Amabile 1983).

It should not be surprising, then, that students for whom rewards are salient — even high-achieving students — will choose the easiest possible tasks (Harter 1978, Green and Lepper 1974).  Commenting on a program sponsored by the Pizza Hut restaurant chain that dangles free pizza before children to induce them to read (“BOOK IT!”), John Nicholls says the likely long-term consequence is “a lot of fat kids who don’t like to read”  (personal communication, 1989).  Children are likely to pick books that are short and simple, the aim being to plow through them fast rather than coming to appreciate the pleasures of reading.  The same is true with respect to inedible extrinsics as well.  Thus, if the question is, Do rewards motivate students?, the answer is Absolutely — they motivate students to get rewarded. Unfortunately, this is often at the expense of interest in, or excellence at, whatever it is they are doing.

All of this prompts several disconcerting questions for anyone committed to CL:  If bribing individuals to learn is so demonstrably ineffective and disadvantageous, what makes us think that bribing groups to learn is productive and benign?  Why, in other words, should CL be exempt from the principle that emerges from this research — viz., The less salient grades and other rewards are for students, the better?  Might it not be naive, in light of the corrosive effect of extrinsics, to assume that we can simply remove the rewards “as soon as the intrinsic motivation inherent in cooperative learning groups becomes apparent” (Johnson, Johnson, and Holubec 1986, p. 63)?

Alternatively, we could frame the challenge this way:  Many of us were drawn to CL because of the manifest failure of competition as a pedagogical tool.  One of the reasons for competition’s failure is precisely its status as an extrinsic motivator (Deci and Ryan 1985, Kohn 1986, Nicholls 1989).  So could it not be said that the use of grades and other rewards to ensure cooperation takes away with one hand what has been given with the other?

To answer these questions definitively, we first need to consider the evidence offered in support of reward-driven CL by such careful researchers as Robert Slavin.  His review of the data has persuaded him that “cooperative learning methods that use specific group rewards based on group members’ individual learning consistently increase achievement more than control methods” (1983, p. 53).  I believe, however, that the force and relevance of this conclusion is sharply limited by several factors.

First, many if not most of the measures in the studies to which Slavin refers are tasks that require only the straightforward application of a known principle (that is, algorithmic or convergent tasks), and these are less vulnerable to the destructive effects of extrinsics than are more open-ended (heuristic or divergent) tasks.  Teachers who care about stimulating creativity and curiosity will not take much comfort from the fact that the promise of a certificate may prompt students to memorize more facts.  That striving for a reward may enhance performance on a boring task may be less important than the finding that rewards turn interesting tasks into boring ones from a student’s perspective.

Second, while Slavin’s notion of methodological adequacy turns in part on whether an experiment lasted for several weeks or several days, we also need to attend to the very long term.  It is true that the toxicity of rewards typically manifests itself with alarming rapidity:  in many of the studies cited above, a single trial — that is, one presentation of an extrinsic reward — was sufficient to undermine performance and interest.  But how do children who are repeatedly bribed to learn come to view the process of learning months or years later?  Specifically, how do they view a given subject when no one is around to reward them?  A temporary performance gain on routine classroom assignments may mask a chronic shift in students’ attitude that will have long-term negative effects on learning.  We already know that “children become increasingly more extrinsically oriented over the school years” (Harter, paraphrased in Barrett and Boggiano 1988; see also Ryan, Connell, and Deci 1985) — a finding that Slavin presumably finds as troubling as I.  It appears likely that the widespread use of extrinsics (mostly by people who have never even heard of cooperative learning) has something to do with this.  Continuing to use extrinsics at the level of the group would seem to be ill-advised.

Third, we need to ask what exactly is being contrasted with reward-driven CL in the studies that find a performance advantage.   My impression is that the control condition typically consists of either (a) a “traditional” classroom, which, as I just noted, is also characterized by reward-based motivation, or (b) some loose, unstructured arrangement (“Why don’t you four work together on this ditto sheet”) that scarcely qualifies as CL.  The first comparison tells us nothing about the effects of rewards per se — only about rewarding individuals versus groups.  The second comparison does nothing to discredit the possibility of carefully structured, nonreward-based approaches to CL.

When Slavin says, as he did in this journal (1988), that “the cooperating groups must have a group goal that is important to them” (p. 31), I heartily agree.  The problem is that he goes on in the very next sentence to operationalize the concept of group goals in terms of “working to earn certificates or other recognition, to receive a few minutes extra of recess, or to earn bonus points on their grades.”

Those of us who are both persuaded and disturbed by all the evidence indicating that such rewards are counterproductive will want to turn to (or create) models of CL that can claim all the familiar advantages but without relying on extrinsics.  This is clearly more difficult than showing why extrinsics should be minimized or abandoned.  I would propose three key components of successful CL by way of starting a discussion that I hope will continue on these pages and elsewhere.  The components that we must bring to, or underscore in, the classroom have to do with curriculum, autonomy, and relationship.

Curriculum obviously matters in many respects, but the point to be emphasized here is that the perceived need to bribe children often tells us more about what they are being asked to learn (namely, that it lacks any intrinsic appeal) than about how learning per se takes place.  While some proponents have proudly described CL as a method that can be used to teach anything — which implies that teachers who adopt it need not ask difficult questions about the value of what they are requiring students to do — others have challenged the value of “using cooperative techniques to have students cover the same boring, inconsequential, or biased material or to have them ‘get through’ worksheets with more efficiency.” (Sapon-Shevin and Schniedewind 1989/1990, p. 64).  I sympathize with the latter point of view.

Autonomy is vital for producing intrinsic motivation because people are more likely to find a task interesting when they have had a role in deciding what they are to do and how they are to do it (Nicholls 1989, Deci and Ryan 1985, Amabile and Gitomer 1984).  Rewards are destructive, in the view of Deci and others, primarily because they restrict autonomy.  But teachers should not only minimize extrinsic motivators; they should affirmatively help students to become responsible for their own education.  A child who can make (teacher-guided) choices about what happens in his or her classroom is a child who will be less likely to require artificial inducements to learn.

Relationship refers to the specific, trainable social skills that already play a part in some models of CL (e.g., Johnson, Johnson, and Holubec 1986) as well as to a broader emphasis on caring for others.  Explicit attention to the value (and intrinsic appeal) of prosocial behavior may encourage students to view others in their group as collaborators rather than as obstacles to their own success.  By contrast, a certain cynicism inheres in the assumption that students will work together only on the basis of self-interest (Kohn 1990) — that is, that no classroom environment could possibly develop norms leading to cooperation without the use of rewards.

Several models of CL already emphasize these things.  First, “if the task is challenging and interesting, and if students are sufficiently prepared for skills in group process, students will experience the process of groupwork itself as highly rewarding,” Elizabeth Cohen (1986, p. 69) has written.  The scores of lessons and activities offered in Schniedewind and Davidson’s (1987) introduction to CL are similarly based on the idea that what gets taught not only matters as much as how it is taught but actually can be the central impetus for learning.  Second, autonomy is key to the Group Investigation approach:  Achievement comes chiefly from giving “students more control over their learning” (Sharan and Sharan 1989/1990, p. 20), not from waving a gradebook at them.

Finally, relationship, and specifically the idea of creating a community within the classroom, is the primary feature of the program developed by the Child Development Project in San Ramon, California.  For that matter, the Project also places special emphasis on the quality of the curriculum and on helping students to take responsibility for their learning — all of which have moved the Project developers away from relying on punishments or rewards (Solomon et al. 1990).

In sum, my hypothesis is that a carefully structured cooperative environment that offers challenging learning tasks, that allows students to make key decisions about how they perform those tasks, and that emphasizes the value (and skills) of helping each other to learn constitutes an alternative to extrinsic motivators that is both more effective over the long haul and more consistent with ideals that most educators share.

But even if we lack certainty about how to make CL work — even if subsequent research modifies this preliminary, three-part formulation — it is time that CL theorists abandoned the project of trying to fine-tune a system of grades and other extrinsic motivators and instead set about trying to maximize the benefits of CL in the absence of rewards.

References

Amabile, T.M. (1983). The Social Psychology of Creativity.  New York: Springer-Verlag.

Amabile, T.M. (1985). “Motivation and Creativity: Effects of Motivational Orientation on Creative Writers.”  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48: 393-399.

Amabile, T.M. and J. Gitomer. (1984). “Children’s Artistic Creativity: Effects of Choice in Task Materials.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 10: 209-215.

Amabile, T.M., B.A. Hennessey, and B.S. Grossman. (1986). “Social Influences on Creativity: The Effects of Contracted-for Reward.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50: 14-23.

Barrett, M., and A.K. Boggiano. (1988). “Fostering Extrinsic Orientations: Use of Reward Strategies to Motivate Children.”  Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 6: 293-309.

Butler, R. (1987). “Task-Involving and Ego-Involving Properties of Evaluation: Effects of Different Feedback Conditions on Motivational Perceptions, Interest, and Performance.”  Journal of Educational Psychology 79: 474-482.

Butler, R. (1988).  “Enhancing and Undermining Intrinsic Motivation: The Effects of Task-Involving and Ego-Involving Evaluation on Interest and Performance.” British Journal of Educational Psychology 58: 1-14.

Butler, R. (1989). “Interest in the Task and Interest in Peers’ Work in Competitive and Noncompetitive Conditions: A Developmental Study.” Child Development 60: 562-570.

Butler, R., and M. Nisan. (1986). “Effects of No Feedback, Task-Related Comments, and Grades on Intrinsic Motivation and Performance.” Journal of Educational Psychology 78: 210-216.

Cohen, E. G.  (1986). Designing Groupwork: Strategies for the Heterogeneous Classroom.  New York:  Teacher College Press.

Condry, J. (1977). “Enemies of Exploration: Self-Initiated Versus Other-Initiated Learning.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35: 459-477.

Deci, E.L., G. Betley, J. Kahle, L. Abrams, and J. Porac. (1981).  ”When Trying to Win: Competition and Intrinsic Motivation.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 7: 79-83.

Deci, E.L., and R.M. Ryan. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior.  New York: Plenum Press.

Deutsch, M. (1949). “A Theory of Cooperation and Competition.” Human Relations 2: 129-152.

Garbarino, J. (1975). “The Impact of Anticipated Reward Upon Cross-Age Tutoring.”  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32: 421-28.

Geller, E.S., J.R. Rudd, M.J. Kalsher, F.M. Streff, and G.R. Lehman. (1987). “Employer-Based Programs to Motivate Safety Belt Use: A Review of Short-Term and Long-Term Effects.” Journal of Safety Research 18: 117.

Greene, D., and M. R. Lepper. (1974).  “Effects of Extrinsic Rewards on Children’s Subsequent Intrinsic Interest.” Child Development 45: 1141-1145.

Harackiewicz, J.M., G. Manderlink, and C. Sansone. (1984).  ”Rewarding Pinball Wizardry: Effects of Evaluation and Cue Value on Intrinsic Interest.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47: 287-300.

Harter, S. (1978). “Pleasure Derived from Challenge and the Effects of Receiving Grades on Children’s Difficulty Level Choices.” Child Development 49: 788-799.

Johnson, D.W., R.T. Johnson, and E.J. Holubec. (1986).  Circles of Learning: Cooperation in the Classroom, revised edition.  Edina, Minn.: Interaction Book Co.

Kohn, A. (1986).  No Contest: The Case Against Competition.  Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Kohn, A. (1990). The Brighter Side of Human Nature: Altruism and Empathy in Everyday Life.  New York: Basic Books.

Kruglanski, A.W., I. Friedman, and G. Zeevi. (1971). “The Effects of Extrinsic Incentive on Some Qualitative Aspects of Task Performance.” Journal of Personality 39: 606-617.

Lepper, M.R., and D. Greene. (April 1975) “When Two Rewards Are Worse than One: Effects of Extrinsic Rewards on Intrinsic Motivation.” Phi Delta Kappan: 565-566.

Lepper, M., and D. Greene, eds. (1978). The Hidden Costs of Reward.  Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lepper, M.R., G. Sagotsky, J.L. Dafoe, and D. Greene. (1982). ”Consequences of Superfluous Social Constraints: Effects on Young Children’s Social Inferences and Subsequent Intrinsic Interest.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology  42: 51-65.

Morgan, M. (1983). “Decrements in Intrinsic Motivation among Rewarded and Observer Subjects.” Child Development 54: 636-644.

Morgan, M. (1984). “Reward-Induced Decrements and Increments in Intrinsic Motivation.” Review of Educational Research 54: 5-30.

Nicholls, J.G. (1989). The Competitive Ethos and Democratic Education.  Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

Ryan, R.M. (1982). “Control and Information in the Intrapersonal Sphere: An Extension of Cognitive Evaluation Theory.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43: 450-461.

Ryan, R.M., J.P. Connell, and E.L. Deci. (1985). “A Motivational Analysis of Self-Determination and Self-Regulation in Education.”  In Research on Motivation in Education, vol. 2, The Classroom Milieu, edited by C. Ames and R. Ames.  Orlando, Fla.: Academic Press.

Sapon-Shevin, M., and N. Schniedewind. (December 1989/January 1990). “Selling Cooperative Learning Without Selling It Short.”  Educational Leadership: 63-65.

Schniedewind, N., and E. Davidson. (1987). Cooperative Learning, Cooperative Lives: A Sourcebook of Learning Activities for Building a Peaceful World. Dubuque, Ia.: William C. Brown Co.

Seligman, C., R.H. Fazio, and M.P. Zanna. (1980). “Effects of Salience of Extrinsic Rewards on Liking and Loving.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 38: 453460.

Sharan, Y., and S. Sharan.  (December 1989/January 1990).  “Group Investigation Expands Cooperative Learning.” Educational Leadership: 17-21.

Slavin, R.E. (1983). Cooperative Learning. New York: Longman.

Slavin, R.E. (October 1988). “Cooperative Learning and Student Achievement.” Educational Leadership: 31-33.

Solomon, D., M. Watson, E. Schaps, V. Battistich, and J. Solomon.   (1990).  “Cooperative Learning as Part of a Comprehensive Classroom Program Designed to Promote Prosocial Development.”  In Cooperative Learning: Theory and Research, edited by Shlomo Sharan.  New York: Praeger.

Sternberg, R.J. (1990). “Prototypes of Competence and Incompetence.”  In Competence Considered, edited by R.J. Sternberg and J. Kolligian, Jr.  New Haven: Yale University Press.

Copyright © 1991 by Alfie Kohn. This article may be downloaded, reproduced, and distributed without permission as long as each copy includes this notice along with citation information (i.e., name of the periodical in which it originally appeared, date of publication, and author’s name). Permission must be obtained in order to reprint this article in a published work or in order to offer it for sale in any form. Please write to the address indicated on the Contact Us page.